• Learning helps tobacco hawkmoths to sele

    From ScienceDaily@1:317/3 to All on Mon Sep 27 21:30:36 2021
    Learning helps tobacco hawkmoths to select a nectar source and
    oviposition site more efficiently

    Date:
    September 27, 2021
    Source:
    Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology
    Summary:
    Researchers have gained new insights into the learning ability
    of tobacco hawkmoths. In two recent publications, they report
    that learning odors does not only play a role in foraging, but
    that female moths are also influenced by previously learned odors
    when choosing a host plant to lay their eggs. In this context,
    a single deposited egg on a certain plant is sufficient for the
    moth's choice to visit the same plant species again even after 24
    hours. In contrast, the moth's second nose, the tip of the proboscis
    on which olfactory sensory cells have also been identified, does
    not appear to play a role in learning odors and making odor-guided
    foraging decisions. The results provide clues to the adaptability
    of these insects to their environment.



    FULL STORY ==========================================================================
    In an earlier study, scientists led by Bill Hansson and Markus Knaden
    from the Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology were already able to
    show that tobacco hawkmoths can learn odors as cues to a nectar source
    that they had initially perceived as unpleasant. The insects had been
    found to be highly adaptive with respect to food sources.


    ==========================================================================
    In the present study, the researchers wanted to know whether learning
    also affects egg-laying. In behavioral experiments, they first gave
    female tobacco hawkmoths the choice of laying their eggs on a leaf of
    either the wild tobacco species Nicotiana attenuata or sacred datura
    Datura wrightii. Nai"ve moths, which had not previously laid an egg on a
    plant, showed an innate preference for the leaves of sacred datura. In a
    second set of experiments, inexperienced females were trained on either
    tobacco or datura leaves, meaning that the moths had no choice but to
    lay their eggs on only one of the two plant species. In a third step,
    the researchers wanted to know whether this training had any effect on selecting one plant over the other. "We were very surprised to observe
    that even a single egg-laying experience was sufficient to direct a
    moth's interest to the very plant on which an egg had previously been
    laid. Tobacco hawkmoths learned this plant even when only one leaf was available for oviposition and the experience was thus decoupled from the
    flower as a nectar source," first author Nandita Nataraj summarizes the results. Apparently, successful oviposition is in itself rewarding to
    the moth and influences future behavior.

    Overall, the experiments revealed that the preference for tobacco
    decreased more significantly after 24 hours than the preference for
    datura, whose leaves they prefer naturally. Innate preference combined
    with success learning appears to be an evolutionary advantage that allows
    the heavy insect, which expends a lot of energy flying and hovering,
    to lay its eggs more efficiently and thereby save resources. It is also
    better protected from predators if it does not have to search for host
    plants for a long time.

    Tobacco hawkmoths learn scents with their antennae, but not with their
    second nose: the tip of their proboscis Tobacco hawkmoths do not only
    learn host plants for more efficient egg laying, they also remember
    flower odors that lead them to a nectar source while foraging. Tobacco hawkmoths detect olfactory cues that guide them to a flower with their antennae. However, in a 2016 study, Markus Knaden's team had found that olfactory neurons on the tip of the proboscis represent a second nose in tobacco hawkmoths. Therefore, the researchers now wanted to know whether
    the moths can also learn odors with this additional olfactory organ.

    "The biggest challenge for our study was the design of an artificial
    flower that retains the odor inside and does not emit it. If the odor
    were also perceptible outside the flower, we would not have been able
    to distinguish between odor learning with the antennae and with the
    proboscis," says first author Elisabeth Adam, describing the special
    feature of the experimental setup. In their natural environment, the crepuscular insects also follow visual cues. The scientists therefore
    developed a flower made of blue acrylic that reliably attracted moths
    even without emitting an odor.

    The behavioral experiments in the wind tunnel revealed that tobacco
    hawkmoths do not learn odors that they perceive only with their
    proboscis. Previous experience with specific odors within the flower
    therefore did not alter foraging behavior. "We know from previous studies
    that higher brain regions are involved in learning. Maybe the role of the proboscis is more that of a tactile or gustatory organ to evaluate the
    quality of a flower. It therefore presumably perceives odors differently
    and does not pass odor information on to higher brain regions," Elisabeth
    Adam explains the results.

    The tobacco hawkmoth Manduca sexta: an ideal model to study the effects of learning on odor-guided behavior in an insect Tobacco hawkmoths exhibit
    many complex behaviors even under laboratory conditions. Following
    a plume indicating flower nectar, or distinguishing between host and
    non-host plants to lay their eggs to ensure that they offspring can
    thrive, are behaviors that are of particular ecological interest.

    "We already know a lot about olfaction in this insect, in particular which brain regions are involved in the transmission and processing of olfactory cues. The moths are therefore ideal models to study odor perception and learning -- from the detection of an odor molecule via neuronal processing
    to the resulting behavior," summarizes study leader Markus Knaden.

    Further studies will now address the questions of whether tobacco
    hawkmoths can also learn to lay their eggs on plants that do not belong
    to their host range and whether they can also learn a variety of different flowers and host plants.

    Additional experiments will also examine how the moth uses the olfactory neurons on its proboscis to evaluate flower odors.

    ========================================================================== Story Source: Materials provided by
    Max_Planck_Institute_for_Chemical_Ecology. Note: Content may be edited
    for style and length.


    ========================================================================== Journal References:
    1. Nandita Nataraj, Elisabeth Adam, Bill S. Hansson, Markus
    Knaden. Host
    Plant Constancy in Ovipositing Manduca sexta. Journal of Chemical
    Ecology, 2021; DOI: 10.1007/s10886-021-01309-3
    2. Elisabeth Adam, Bill S. Hansson, Markus Knaden. Moths sense but
    do not
    learn flower odors with their proboscis during flower investigation.

    Journal of Experimental Biology, 2021; 224 (17) DOI:
    10.1242/jeb.242780 ==========================================================================

    Link to news story: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/09/210927102602.htm

    --- up 3 weeks, 4 days, 8 hours, 25 minutes
    * Origin: -=> Castle Rock BBS <=- Now Husky HPT Powered! (1:317/3)